Theory of Mind Understanding and Socioeconomic Status

Research Article

J Psychiatry Mental Disord. 2024; 9(1): 1075.

Theory of Mind Understanding and Socioeconomic Status

Nazila Shojaeian*

Department of Clinical and Health, Psychology, Universitat Autònoma de, Barcelona, Spain

*Corresponding author: Nazila Shojaeian Department of Clinical and Health, Psychology, Universitat Autònoma de, Barcelona, Spain. Email: nazila.shojaeian@gmail.com

Received: January 29, 2024 Accepted: March 05, 2024 Published: March 12, 2024

Abstract

The participants underwent assessments across all Theory of Mind (ToM) tasks. All individuals were native speakers, comprising 74 from Iran and 66 from Sweden, aged 6-12, and included those with autism spectrum disorder, Down syndrome, and typical development. We conclude that children’s behavior serves as a scale for social and thought problem assessments, evaluated based on teachers’ and parents’ scores. These scales exhibited a robust correlation with ToM results as perceived by teachers but diverged significantly from parents’ perspectives. While exploring the relationship between family socioeconomic status and children’s ToM understanding, we were unable to establish a clear link in any of our cases. The current findings underscore that the impact of culture is partially evident in the specific tasks developed by children during childhood. However, this cultural influence did not extend to the entire ToM construct. In essence, our results indicate that while the effect of culture is partially confirmed in certain tasks developed by children in their formative years, it is not uniformly present across the entirety of the ToM constructs within these groups.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorder; Down syndrome; Intelligence quotient; Theory of Mind; Socioeconomic status, ASEBA

Introduction

According to Dunn et al. (1991) [1], the social world plays a mediating role in crucial conceptual advancements evident in social cognition tasks. Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) often encounter challenges in social communication and interactions [2]. Consequently, children with ASD may find it difficult to express themselves socially and communicate effectively with strangers, thereby affecting their overall social and communication skills. To test their theory, Leslie and Frith (1988) designed dolls, and the experiment was subsequently replicated by real individuals who enacted scenarios involving these dolls [3].

Method

Participants

The initial sample consisted of 155 children: 86 from Iran and 69 from Sweden. After excluding 140 schoolchildren from Tehran and Karaj, Iran, the study focused on 74 children (43 boys and 31 girls), including 24 with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), 24 with Down Dyndrome (DS), and 26 with Typical Development (TD). Additionally, 66 children (33 boys and 33 girls) aged 6-12, with similar characteristics were included, comprising 26 with ASD, 18 with DS, and 22 with TD. Sampling was conducted across 23 different locations in Tehran, including clinics, centers, and elementary schools, with and without special needs. In Sweden, children with Typical Development (TD), Down Syndrome (DS), and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) are selected from regular schools in Stockholm and Göteborg. Children with Down syndrome was recruited from Särskolan schools, which were designed for typically developing students.

Procedures

To gather medical, behavioral, and psychological information about the children in our clinical sample, we used a comprehensive approach. Parents were requested to complete a Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) questionnaire that offered insights into their children’s behavior. Simultaneously, the teachers provided corresponding information by filling out the Teacher Report Form (TRF), detailing the child's behavior in the school environment. As a native speaker of Persian (Farsi), the author administered all tasks to Iranian individuals in a language familiar to them. Swedish children in similar circumstances underwent testing either at their schools or homes facilitated by a native Swedish research assistant. The testing process involved individual sessions, conducted either by the author or in collaboration with a local researcher, in a quiet room within the school or clinic setting.

Tasks and Instruments:

COGNITIVE MEASURE: Raven's Progressive Matrices (RPMs)

John C. Raven (2002) developed Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPMs), originally introduced by Raven in 1936. Colored Progressive Matrices (CPM) and Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM) were employed for both typical and clinical groups, with SPM specifically used for children typically aged 12 years. Identical versions of Raven's Progressive Matrices were used to establish IQ ranges for both Iranian and Swedish children [4,5].

In the absence of a Swedish standardization version, British norms were applied in Sweden [6]. It is worth noting that the Raven test had been standardized with Iranian individuals in previous studies [7].

Sally and Anne Task; Baron-Cohen, Leslie & Frith (1985): This classic task evaluates an individual's social cognitive ability within the first order of Theory of Mind (ToM) (1985).

Smarties Tube Task; Perner, Leekam, and Wimmer (1987) [9]; Perner et al. (1989) [3]: The Smarties task, developed by Perner and Wimmer (1987) [9], involves a tube containing smarties instead of a pen. The child demonstrates and reseals the box to reveal its contents.

Representational Change Test (Picture Task); Gopnik & Astington (1988) [10]: In this task by Gopnik and Astington (1988), children initially view animal pictures in various colors. Subsequently, the examiner disclosed that the animals were concealed except for one body part. With the exception of the final picture and some false belief questions, these are the same objects that the children encountered earlier.

The New Theory of Mind (ToM) Test; Muris et al., (1999) [11]: For the current study, we utilized the formatted version by Karen L. Anderson (2013), comprising 20 items for ToM 1 (scored from 0 to 20), 13 items for ToM 2 (scored from 0 to 13), and 5 items for ToM 3 (scored from 0 to 5).

Behavior: The TRF and CBCL Behavior Scales 6/18; T. M. Achenbach (1991) [12]: The Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA) employs rating forms to evaluate competencies and problems in an individual's behavior from various sources. Our references were parents or parent surrogates using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL 6/18), and teachers or school personnel using the Teacher Report Form (TRF 6/18) [12]. Each item was rated on a three-point Likert scale regarding its applicability to the child: 0 = not true, 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, 2 = very true or often true, or within the past six months for the CBCL and more than two months for the TRF [12]. The questionnaires were completed by parents in both countries in their respective languages, with the Iranian versions documented in earlier studies (Minaei 2005, Dadsetan, Bayat, et al. 2010) and the Swedish versions (Larsson and Frisk 1999, Achenbach and Rescorla 2000) [13,14].

MEASURE OF SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS: The Hollingshead 4 Factor Index of Socioeconomic Status (SES); Hollingshead (1975): The Hollingshead four-factor index of Socioeconomic Status (SES) is a survey designed to assess family socioeconomic status [15]. This measure incorporates social status determined by parents’ educational level, profession, and occupational status, which are code-rated on predetermined scales. The education code was scored on a 7-point scale, and the occupational code was scored on a 9-point scale. These codes were then multiplied by specific values (profession and occupation level 5 + education 3 = divided by 2 if there were two parents).

Data Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS version 25 (IBM Corporation, 2013). Descriptive statistics were employed to examine the participants' sociodemographic characteristics and variables. Furthermore, analysis of variance (ANOVA) was utilized to explore group differences in Theory of Mind (ToM) tasks, providing mean and standard deviation values for quantitative variables, and frequency and prevalence for categorical variables. The rates of the measures were evaluated through ANOVA, and post-hoc comparisons were carried out using Bonferroni tests. Additionally, one-way ANOVA was applied to assess the association between variables. The relationship between ASEBA internalization and externalization indicators and socioeconomic status was determined using analysis of variance and correlation coefficients. The significance level was set at .05.

Results

Based on evaluations from both parents and teachers, the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA) was used to assess internalizing and externalizing disorders. Our findings demonstrated a robust correlation between our thought and social problem scales and Theory of Mind (ToM) scores. Notably, there were significant differences in Teacher Report Form (TRF) scores for social problems, thought problems, internalizing, and externalizing, providing additional support for the majority of the tests. Conversely, there was no significant difference in any variable among individuals who completed the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) questionnaire.

The use of ToM tasks aimed to gauge the impact of a country, revealing a relationship specific to certain tasks rather than ToM as a whole. Specifically, the Sally and Anne tasks, including S_A_R (2 (1) = 1.550; P =.213), S_A_M (2 (1) = 1.512; P =.219), and S_A_C (2 (1) = .125; P =.724), did not exhibit a significant relationship with the country.

Furthermore, significant differences were observed between the Smart_FT and Smart_RQ subscales ((1) = 7.553 and 27.296, respectively). Smart_N scores consistently yielded positive results ((1) = .906; P =.341). Regarding false beliefs and representational change (Repr_FB), no significant relationship with the country was found ((1) = .034; P =.853). However, both the Representational Change-Question (Repr_Q) scores ((1) = 7.038; P =.008) and the Representational Change-Reality (Repr_RD) scores ((1) = 23.993; P < .001) exhibited significant relationships with countries. Several orders of the New Theory of Mind task did not show any significant relationship with the NTT_1 (T = .296; p = .441), NTT_2 (T = .996, p = .743), and NTT_3 subscales (T = 2.325; p = .508). For further details, refer to Table 1.

Citation:Shojaeian N. Theory of Mind Understanding and Socioeconomic Status. J Psychiatry Mental Disord. 2024; 9(1): 1075.